Epidemiology is the science that studies the patterns,
causes, and effects of
health and
disease conditions in defined
populations. It is the cornerstone of
public health, and informs policy decisions and
evidence-based practice by identifying
risk factors for disease and targets for
preventive healthcare. Epidemiologists help with study design, collection, and
statistical analysis of data, and interpretation and dissemination of results (including
peer review and occasional
systematic review). Epidemiology has helped develop
methodology used in
clinical research,
public health studies, and, to a lesser extent,
basic research in the biological sciences.
[1]
Etymology[edit]
Epidemiology, literally meaning "the study of what is upon the people", is derived from
Greek epi, meaning "upon, among",
demos, meaning "people, district", and
logos, meaning "study, word, discourse", suggesting that it applies only to human populations. However, the term is widely used in studies of zoological populations (veterinary epidemiology), although the term "
epizoology" is available, and it has also been applied to studies of plant populations (botanical or
plant disease epidemiology).
[2]
The distinction between "epidemic" and "endemic" was first drawn by
Hippocrates,
[3] to distinguish between diseases that are "visited upon" a population (epidemic) from those that "reside within" a population (endemic).
[4] The term "epidemiology" appears to have first been used to describe the study of epidemics in 1802 by the Spanish physician Villalba in
Epidemiología Española.
[4] Epidemiologists also study the interaction of diseases in a population, a condition known as a
syndemic.
The term epidemiology is now widely applied to cover the description and causation of not only epidemic disease, but of disease in general, and even many non-disease health-related conditions, such as high blood pressure and obesity. Therefore, this epidemiology is based upon how the pattern of the disease cause changes in the function of everyone.
History[edit]
"History of epidemiology" redirects here.
The Greek physician
Hippocrates, known as the father of medicine,
[5][6] sought a logic to sickness; he is the first person known to have examined the relationships between the occurrence of disease and environmental influences.
[7] Hippocrates believed sickness of the human body to be caused by an imbalance of the four
humors (air, fire, water and earth “atoms”). The cure to the sickness was to remove or add the humor in question to balance the body. This belief led to the application of bloodletting and dieting in medicine.
[8] He coined the terms
endemic (for diseases usually found in some places but not in others) and
epidemic (for diseases that are seen at some times but not others).
[9]
In ancient India,
Ayurveda considered disease to be a manifestation of imbalance in 3 bodily humors, called
doshass. Around this theory, systems of
diagnosis were based.
One of the earliest theories on the origin of disease was that it was primarily the fault of human luxury. This was expressed by philosophers such as
Plato[10] and
Rousseau,
[11]and social critics like
Jonathan Swift.
[12]
In the middle of the 16th century, a doctor from
Verona named
Girolamo Fracastoro was the first to propose a theory that these very small, unseeable, particles that cause disease were alive. They were considered to be able to spread by air, multiply by themselves and to be destroyable by fire. In this way he refuted
Galen's
miasma theory (poison gas in sick people). In 1543 he wrote a book
De contagione et contagiosis morbis, in which he was the first to promote personal and environmental
hygiene to prevent disease. The development of a sufficiently powerful microscope by
Anton van Leeuwenhoek in 1675 provided visual evidence of living particles consistent with a
germ theory of disease.
Another pioneer,
Thomas Sydenham (1624–1689), was the first to distinguish the fevers of Londoners in the later 1600s. His theories on cures of fevers met with much resistance from traditional physicians at the time. He was not able to find the initial cause of the
smallpox fever he researched and treated.
[8]
John Graunt, a
haberdasher and amateur statistician, published
Natural and Political Observations ... upon the Bills of Mortality in 1662. In it, he analysed the mortality rolls in
London before the
Great Plague, presented one of the first
life tables, and report time trends for many diseases, new and old. He provided statistical evidence for many theories on disease, and also refuted some widespread ideas on them.
Modern era[edit]
John Snow is famous for his investigations into the causes of the 19th century cholera epidemics, and is also known as the father of (modern) epidemiology.
[13][14] He began with noticing the significantly higher death rates in two areas supplied by Southwark Company. His identification of the
Broad Street pump as the cause of the Soho epidemic is considered the classic example of epidemiology. Snow used chlorine in an attempt to clean the water and removed the handle; this ended the outbreak. This has been perceived as a major event in the history of
public health and regarded as the founding event of the science of epidemiology, having helped shape public health policies around the world.
[15][16] However, Snow’s research and preventive measures to avoid further outbreaks were not fully accepted or put into practice until after his death.
In the late 20th century, with advancement of biomedical sciences, a number of molecular markers in blood, other biospecimens and environment were identified as predictors of development or risk of a certain disease. Epidemiology research to examine the relationship between these
biomarkers analyzed at the molecular level and disease was broadly named “
molecular epidemiology”. Specifically, "
genetic epidemiology" has been used for epidemiology of germline genetic variation and disease. Genetic variation is typically determined using DNA from peripheral blood leukocytes. Since the 2000s,
genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have been commonly performed to identify genetic risk factors for many diseases and health conditions.
While most molecular epidemiology studies are still using conventional disease
diagnosis and classification systems, it is increasingly recognized that disease
evolutionrepresents inherently heterogeneous processes differing from person to person. Conceptually, each individual has a unique disease process different from any other individual (“the unique disease principle”),
[23][24] considering uniqueness of the
exposome (a totality of endogenous and exogenous / environmental exposures) and its unique influence on molecular pathologic process in each individual. Studies to examine the relationship between an exposure and molecular pathologic signature of disease (particularly
cancer) became increasingly common throughout the 2000s. However, the use of
molecular pathology in epidemiology posed unique challenges including lack of research guidelines and standardized
statistical methodologies, and paucity of interdisciplinary experts and training programs.
[25] Furthermore, the concept of disease heterogeneity appears to conflict with the long-standing premise in epidemiology that individuals with the same disease name have similar etiologies and disease processes. To resolve these issues and advance population health science in the era of molecular
precision medicine, “
molecular pathology” and “epidemiology” was integrated to create a new interdisciplinary field of “
molecular pathological epidemiology” (MPE),
[26][27] defined as “epidemiology of
molecular pathology and heterogeneity of disease”. In MPE, investigators analyze the relationships between; (A) environmental, dietary, lifestyle and genetic factors; (B) alterations in cellular or extracellular molecules; and (C) evolution and progression of disease. A better understanding of heterogeneity of disease
pathogenesis will further contribute to elucidate
etiologies of disease. The MPE approach can be applied to not only neoplastic diseases but also non-neoplastic diseases.
[28] The concept and paradigm of MPE have become widespread in the 2010s.
[29][30][31][32][33][34][35]
The profession[edit]
To date, few
universities offer epidemiology as a course of study at the undergraduate level. Many epidemiologists are
physicians, or hold graduate degrees such as a
Master of Public Health (MPH),
Master of Science of Epidemiology (MSc.).
Doctorates include the
Doctor of Public Health (DrPH),
Doctor of Pharmacy (PharmD),
Doctor of Philosophy(PhD),
Doctor of Science (ScD),
Doctor of Social Work (DSW),
Doctor of Clinical Practice (DClinP),
Doctor of Podiatric Medicine (DPM),
Doctor of Veterinary Medicine (DVM),
Doctor of Nursing Practice (DNP),
Doctor of Physical Therapy (DPT), or for clinically trained physicians,
Doctor of Medicine (MD) or
Bachelor of Medicine and Surgery (MBBS or MBChB) and
Doctor of Osteopathic Medicine (DO).
As public health/health protection practitioners, epidemiologists work in a number of different settings. Some epidemiologists work 'in the field'; i.e., in the community, commonly in a public health/health protection service and are often at the forefront of investigating and combating disease outbreaks. Others work for non-profit organizations, universities, hospitals and larger government entities such as the
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), the
Health Protection Agency, the
World Health Organization (WHO), or the
Public Health Agency of Canada. Epidemiologists can also work in for-profit organizations such as pharmaceutical and medical device companies in groups such as market research or clinical development.
The practice[edit]
Epidemiologists employ a range of study designs from the observational to experimental and generally categorized as descriptive, analytic (aiming to further examine known associations or hypothesized relationships), and experimental (a term often equated with clinical or community trials of treatments and other interventions). In observational studies, nature is allowed to “take its course”, as epidemiologists observe from the sidelines. Conversely, in experimental studies, the epidemiologist is the one in control of all of the factors entering a certain case study.
[36] Epidemiological studies are aimed, where possible, at revealing unbiased relationships between
exposures such as alcohol or smoking,
biological agents,
stress, or
chemicals to
mortality or
morbidity. The identification of causal relationships between these exposures and outcomes is an important aspect of epidemiology. Modern epidemiologists use
informatics as a tool.
Observational studies have two components: descriptive, or analytical. Descriptive observations pertain to the “who, what, where and when of health-related state occurrence”. However, analytical observations deal more with the ‘how’ of a health-related event.
[36]
Experimental epidemiology contains three case types: randomized control trial (often used for new medicine or drug testing), field trial (conducted on those at a high risk of conducting a disease), and community trial (research on social originating diseases).
[36]
Unfortunately, many epidemiology studies conducted cause false or misinterpreted information to circulate the public. According to an epidemiology class taught by professor Madhukar Pai at McGill, “...optimism bias is pervasive, most studies biased or inconclusive or false, most discovered true associations are inflated, fear and panic inducing rather than helpful; media-induced panic, cannot detect small effects; big effects are not to be found anymore”.
[37]
The term 'epidemiologic triad' is used to describe the intersection of Host, Agent, and Environment in analyzing an outbreak.
As causal inference[edit]
Although epidemiology is sometimes viewed as a collection of statistical tools used to elucidate the associations of exposures to health outcomes, a deeper understanding of this science is that of discovering causal relationships.
"
Correlation does not imply causation" is a common theme for much of the epidemiological literature. For epidemiologists, the key is in the term
inference. Epidemiologists use gathered data and a broad range of biomedical and psychosocial theories in an iterative way to generate or expand theory, to test hypotheses, and to make educated, informed assertions about which relationships are causal, and about exactly how they are causal.
Epidemiologists Rothman and Greenland emphasize that the "one cause – one effect" understanding is a simplistic mis-belief. Most outcomes, whether disease or death, are caused by a chain or web consisting of many component causes. Causes can be distinguished as necessary, sufficient or probabilistic conditions. If a necessary condition can be identified and controlled (e.g., antibodies to a disease agent), the harmful outcome can be avoided.
Bradford Hill criteria[edit]
In 1965
Austin Bradford Hill proposed a series of considerations to help assess evidence of causation,
[38] which have come to be commonly known as the "
Bradford Hill criteria". In contrast to the explicit intentions of their author, Hill's considerations are now sometimes taught as a checklist to be implemented for assessing causality.
[39] Hill himself said "None of my nine viewpoints can bring indisputable evidence for or against the cause-and-effect hypothesis and none can be required
sine qua non."
[38]
- Strength: A small association does not mean that there is not a causal effect, though the larger the association, the more likely that it is causal.[38]
- Consistency: Consistent findings observed by different persons in different places with different samples strengthens the likelihood of an effect.[38]
- Specificity: Causation is likely if a very specific population at a specific site and disease with no other likely explanation. The more specific an association between a factor and an effect is, the bigger the probability of a causal relationship.[38]
- Temporality: The effect has to occur after the cause (and if there is an expected delay between the cause and expected effect, then the effect must occur after that delay).[38]
- Biological gradient: Greater exposure should generally lead to greater incidence of the effect. However, in some cases, the mere presence of the factor can trigger the effect. In other cases, an inverse proportion is observed: greater exposure leads to lower incidence.[38]
- Plausibility: A plausible mechanism between cause and effect is helpful (but Hill noted that knowledge of the mechanism is limited by current knowledge).[38]
- Coherence: Coherence between epidemiological and laboratory findings increases the likelihood of an effect. However, Hill noted that "... lack of such [laboratory] evidence cannot nullify the epidemiological effect on associations".[38]
- Experiment: "Occasionally it is possible to appeal to experimental evidence".[38]
- Analogy: The effect of similar factors may be considered.[38]
Legal interpretation[edit]
Epidemiological studies can only go to prove that an agent could have caused, but not that it did cause, an effect in any particular case:
"Epidemiology is concerned with the
incidence of disease in populations and does not address the question of the cause of an individual's disease. This question, sometimes referred to as specific causation, is beyond the domain of the science of epidemiology. Epidemiology has its limits at the point where an inference is made that the relationship between an agent and a disease is causal (general causation) and where the magnitude of excess risk attributed to the agent has been determined; that is, epidemiology addresses whether an agent can cause a disease, not whether an agent did cause a specific plaintiff's disease."
[40]
In United States law, epidemiology alone cannot prove that a causal association does not exist in general. Conversely, it can be (and is in some circumstances) taken by US courts, in an individual case, to justify an inference that a causal association does exist, based upon a balance of
probability.
The subdiscipline of forensic epidemiology is directed at the investigation of specific causation of disease or injury in individuals or groups of individuals in instances in which causation is disputed or is unclear, for presentation in legal settings.
Advocacy[edit]
As a
public health discipline, epidemiologic evidence is often used to
advocate both personal measures like diet change and corporate measures like removal of
junk foodadvertising, with study findings disseminated to the general public to help people to make informed decisions about their health. Often the uncertainties about these findings are not communicated well; news articles often prominently report the latest result of one study with little mention of its limitations, caveats, or context. Epidemiological tools have proved effective in establishing major causes of diseases like
cholera and
lung cancer,
[38] but experience difficulty in regards to more subtle health issues where causation is more complex. Notably, conclusions drawn from observational studies may be reconsidered as later data from
randomized controlled trials becomes available, as was the case with the association between the use of
hormone replacement therapy and cardiac risk.
[41]
Population-based health management[edit]
Epidemiological practice and the results of epidemiological analysis make a significant contribution to emerging population-based health management frameworks.
Population-based health management encompasses the ability to:
- Assess the health states and health needs of a target population;
- Implement and evaluate interventions that are designed to improve the health of that population; and
- Efficiently and effectively provide care for members of that population in a way that is consistent with the community's cultural, policy and health resource values.
Modern population-based health management is complex, requiring a multiple set of skills (medical, political, technological, mathematical etc.) of which epidemiological practice and analysis is a core component, that is unified with management science to provide efficient and effective health care and health guidance to a population. This task requires the forward looking ability of modern risk management approaches that transform health risk factors, incidence, prevalence and mortality statistics (derived from epidemiological analysis) into management metrics that not only guide how a health system responds to current population health issues, but also how a health system can be managed to better respond to future potential population health issues.
[42]
Examples of organizations that use population-based health management that leverage the work and results of epidemiological practice include Canadian Strategy for Cancer Control, Health Canada Tobacco Control Programs, Rick Hansen Foundation, Canadian Tobacco Control Research Initiative.
[43][44][45]
Each of these organizations use a population-based health management framework called Life at Risk that combines epidemiological quantitative analysis with demographics, health agency operational research and economics to perform:
- Population Life Impacts Simulations: Measurement of the future potential impact of disease upon the population with respect to new disease cases, prevalence, premature death as well as potential years of life lost from disability and death;
- Labour Force Life Impacts Simulations: Measurement of the future potential impact of disease upon the labour force with respect to new disease cases, prevalence, premature death and potential years of life lost from disability and death;
- Economic Impacts of Disease Simulations: Measurement of the future potential impact of disease upon private sector disposable income impacts (wages, corporate profits, private health care costs) and public sector disposable income impacts (personal income tax, corporate income tax, consumption taxes, publicly funded health care costs).
Types of studies[edit]
Main article:
Study design
Case series[edit]
Case-series may refer to the qualitative study of the experience of a single patient, or small group of patients with a similar diagnosis, or to a statistical technique comparing periods during which patients are exposed to some factor with the potential to produce illness with periods when they are unexposed.
The former type of study is purely descriptive and cannot be used to make inferences about the general population of patients with that disease. These types of studies, in which an astute clinician identifies an unusual feature of a disease or a patient's history, may lead to formulation of a new hypothesis. Using the data from the series, analytic studies could be done to investigate possible causal factors. These can include case control studies or prospective studies. A case control study would involve matching comparable controls without the disease to the cases in the series. A prospective study would involve following the case series over time to evaluate the disease's natural history.
[46]
The latter type, more formally described as self-controlled case-series studies, divide individual patient follow-up time into exposed and unexposed periods and use fixed-effects Poisson regression processes to compare the incidence rate of a given outcome between exposed and unexposed periods. This technique has been extensively used in the study of adverse reactions to vaccination, and has been shown in some circumstances to provide statistical power comparable to that available in cohort studies.
Case-control studies[edit]
Case-control studies select subjects based on their disease status. It is a retrospective study. A group of individuals that are disease positive (the "case" group) is compared with a group of disease negative individuals (the "control" group). The control group should ideally come from the same population that gave rise to the cases. The case-control study looks back through time at potential exposures that both groups (cases and controls) may have encountered. A 2×2 table is constructed, displaying exposed cases (A), exposed controls (B), unexposed cases (C) and unexposed controls (D). The statistic generated to measure association is the
odds ratio (OR), which is the ratio of the odds of exposure in the cases (A/C) to the odds of exposure in the controls (B/D), i.e. OR = (AD/BC).
| Cases | Controls |
Exposed | A | B |
Unexposed | C | D |
If the OR is clearly greater than 1, then the conclusion is "those with the disease are more likely to have been exposed," whereas if it is close to 1 then the exposure and disease are not likely associated. If the OR is far less than one, then this suggests that the exposure is a protective factor in the causation of the disease. Case-control studies are usually faster and more cost effective than
cohort studies, but are sensitive to bias (such as
recall bias and
selection bias). The main challenge is to identify the appropriate control group; the distribution of exposure among the control group should be representative of the distribution in the population that gave rise to the cases. This can be achieved by drawing a random sample from the original population at risk. This has as a consequence that the control group can contain people with the disease under study when the disease has a high attack rate in a population.
A major drawback for case control studies is that, in order to be considered to be statistically significant, the minimum number of cases required at the 95% confidence interval is related to the odds ratio by the equation:
total cases = (a+c) = (1.96)^2×(1+N)×(1÷ln(OR))^2×((OR+2√OR+1)÷√OR)≈15.5×(1+N)×(1÷ln(OR))^2
where N = the ratio of cases to controls. As the odds ratio approached 1, approaches 0; rendering case control studies all but useless for low odds ratios. For instance, for an odds ratio of 1.5 and cases = controls, the table shown above would look like this:
..... | Cases | Controls |
Exposed | 103 | 84 |
Unexposed | 84 | 103 |
For an odds ratio of 1.1:
..... | Cases | Controls |
Exposed | 1732 | 1652 |
Unexposed | 1652 | 1732 |
Cohort studies[edit]
Cohort studies select subjects based on their exposure status. The study subjects should be at risk of the outcome under investigation at the beginning of the cohort study; this usually means that they should be disease free when the cohort study starts. The cohort is followed through time to assess their later outcome status. An example of a cohort study would be the investigation of a cohort of smokers and non-smokers over time to estimate the incidence of lung cancer. The same 2×2 table is constructed as with the case control study. However, the point estimate generated is the
relative risk (RR), which is the probability of disease for a person in the exposed group,
Pe =
A / (
A +
B) over the probability of disease for a person in the unexposed group,
Pu =
C / (
C +
D), i.e.
RR =
Pe /
Pu.
..... | Case | Non-case | Total |
Exposed | A | B | (A + B) |
Unexposed | C | D | (C + D) |
As with the OR, a RR greater than 1 shows association, where the conclusion can be read "those with the exposure were more likely to develop disease."
Prospective studies have many benefits over case control studies. The RR is a more powerful effect measure than the OR, as the OR is just an estimation of the RR, since true incidence cannot be calculated in a case control study where subjects are selected based on disease status. Temporality can be established in a prospective study, and confounders are more easily controlled for. However, they are more costly, and there is a greater chance of losing subjects to follow-up based on the long time period over which the cohort is followed.
Cohort studies also are limited by the same equation for number of cases as for cohort studies, but, if the base incidence rate in the study population is very low, the number of cases required is reduced by ½.
Outbreak investigation[edit]
- For information on investigation of infectious disease outbreaks, please see outbreak investigation.
Validity: precision and bias[edit]
Different fields in epidemiology have different levels of validity. One way to assess the validity of findings is the ratio of false-positives (claimed effects that are not correct) to false-negatives (studies which fail to support a true effect). To take the field of genetic epidemiology, candidate-gene studies produced over 100 false-positive findings for each false-negative. By contrast genome-wide association appear close to the reverse, with only one false positive for every 100 or more false-negatives.
[47] This ratio has improved over time in genetic epidemiology as the field has adopted stringent criteria. By contrast other epidemiological fields have not required such rigorous reporting and are much less reliable as a result.
[47]
Random error[edit]
Random error is the result of fluctuations around a true value because of sampling variability. Random error is just that: random. It can occur during data collection, coding, transfer, or analysis. Examples of random error include: poorly worded questions, a misunderstanding in interpreting an individual answer from a particular respondent, or a typographical error during coding. Random error affects measurement in a transient, inconsistent manner and it is impossible to correct for random error.
There is random error in all sampling procedures. This is called
sampling error.
Precision in epidemiological variables is a measure of random error. Precision is also inversely related to random error, so that to reduce random error is to increase precision. Confidence intervals are computed to demonstrate the precision of relative risk estimates. The narrower the confidence interval, the more precise the relative risk estimate.
There are two basic ways to reduce random error in an
epidemiological study. The first is to increase the sample size of the study. In other words, add more subjects to your study. The second is to reduce the variability in measurement in the study. This might be accomplished by using a more precise measuring device or by increasing the number of measurements.
Note, that if sample size or number of measurements are increased, or a more precise measuring tool is purchased, the costs of the study are usually increased. There is usually an uneasy balance between the need for adequate precision and the practical issue of study cost.
Systematic error[edit]
A systematic error or bias occurs when there is a difference between the true value (in the population) and the observed value (in the study) from any cause other than sampling variability. An example of systematic error is if, unknown to you, the
pulse oximeter you are using is set incorrectly and adds two points to the true value each time a measurement is taken. The measuring device could be
precise but not accurate. Because the error happens in every instance, it is systematic. Conclusions you draw based on that data will still be incorrect. But the error can be reproduced in the future (e.g., by using the same mis-set instrument).
A mistake in coding that affects all responses for that particular question is another example of a systematic error.
The validity of a study is dependent on the degree of systematic error. Validity is usually separated into two components:
- Internal validity is dependent on the amount of error in measurements, including exposure, disease, and the associations between these variables. Good internal validity implies a lack of error in measurement and suggests that inferences may be drawn at least as they pertain to the subjects under study.
- External validity pertains to the process of generalizing the findings of the study to the population from which the sample was drawn (or even beyond that population to a more universal statement). This requires an understanding of which conditions are relevant (or irrelevant) to the generalization. Internal validity is clearly a prerequisite for external validity.
Three types of bias[edit]
Selection bias[edit]
Selection bias is one of three types of bias that can threaten the validity of a study. Selection bias occurs when study subjects are selected or become part of the study as a result of a third, unmeasured variable which is associated with both the exposure and outcome of interest.
[48] For instance, it has repeatedly been noted that cigarette smokers and non smokers tend to differ in their study participation rates. (Sackett D cites the example of Seltzer et al., in which 85% of non smokers and 67% of smokers returned mailed questionnaires.)
[49] It is important to note that such a difference in response will not lead to bias if it is not also associated with a systematic difference in outcome between the two response groups.
Information bias[edit]
Information bias is bias arising from systematic error in the assessment of a variable.
[50] An example of this is recall bias. A typical example is again provided by Sackett in his discussion of a study examining the effect of specific exposures on fetal health: "in questioning mothers whose recent pregnancies had ended in fetal death or malformation (cases) and a matched group of mothers whose pregnancies ended normally (controls) it was found that 28% of the former, but only 20% of the latter, reported exposure to drugs which could not be substantiated either in earlier prospective interviews or in other health records".
[49] In this example, recall bias probably occurred as a result of women who had had miscarriages having an apparent tendency to better recall and therefore report previous exposures.
Confounding[edit]
Confounding has traditionally been defined as bias arising from the co-occurrence or mixing of effects of extraneous factors, referred to as confounders, with the main effect(s) of interest.
[50][51] A more recent definition of confounding invokes the notion of
counterfactual effects.
[51] According to this view, when one observes an outcome of interest, say Y=1 (as opposed to Y=0), in a given population A which is entirely exposed (i.e. exposure
X = 1 for every unit of the population) the risk of this event will be
RA1. The counterfactual or unobserved risk
RA0 corresponds to the risk which would have been observed if these same individuals had been unexposed (i.e.
X = 0 for every unit of the population). The true effect of exposure therefore is:
RA1 −
RA0 (if one is interested in risk differences) or
RA1/
RA0 (if one is interested in relative risk). Since the counterfactual risk
RA0 is unobservable we approximate it using a second population B and we actually measure the following relations:
RA1 −
RB0 or
RA1/
RB0. In this situation, confounding occurs when
RA0 ≠
RB0.
[51](NB: Example assumes binary outcome and exposure variables.)
Some epidemiologists prefer to think of confounding separately from common categorizations of bias since, unlike selection and information bias, confounding stems from real causal effects.
[48]
Journals[edit]
General journals:
|
Specialty journals:
|
By physiology/disease:
|
By methodological approach:
|
Post a Comment
Click to see the code!
To insert emoticon you must added at least one space before the code.